Breaking News
February 17, 2019 - Exercise gives a better brain boost to older men than women
February 17, 2019 - New research disproves previous assumptions of how looks influence personality
February 17, 2019 - Cannabis use as a teenager linked to depression later in life
February 17, 2019 - Sinks by Toilets in ICU Patient Rooms Harbor Harmful Bacteria
February 17, 2019 - Cancer cells’ plasticity makes them harder to stop
February 17, 2019 - Young cannabis users have increased risk of depression and suicidal behavior
February 17, 2019 - Tasmanian Devils Likely to Survive Cancer Scourge
February 17, 2019 - Neoadjuvant PD-1 blockade seems effective in glioblastoma
February 17, 2019 - Personal, social factors play role in enabling sustainable return to work after ill health
February 17, 2019 - Mouse studies show ‘inhibition’ theory of autism wrong
February 17, 2019 - Study shows how neuroactive steroids inhibit activity of pro-inflammatory proteins
February 17, 2019 - Use of liver grafts from older donors decreased despite better outcomes in recipients
February 17, 2019 - MUSC researchers discover new mechanism for a class of anti-cancer drugs
February 17, 2019 - HPV misconceptions are causing women to miss smear tests
February 17, 2019 - Sanofi and Regeneron Offer Praluent (alirocumab) at a New Reduced U.S. List Price
February 17, 2019 - Researchers say auditory testing can identify children for autism screening
February 17, 2019 - New method analyzes how single biological cells react to stressful situations
February 17, 2019 - WVU gynecologic oncologist investigates novel treatment for cervical and vaginal cancers
February 17, 2019 - ADHD diagnoses poorly documented
February 17, 2019 - Majority of gender minority youth do not identify with traditional sexual identity labels
February 17, 2019 - AbbVie, Teneobio enter into strategic transaction to develop potential treatment for multiple myeloma
February 17, 2019 - Lower Birth Weight May Up Risk for Psychiatric Disorders
February 17, 2019 - Scientists identify reversible molecular defect underlying rheumatoid arthritis
February 17, 2019 - Moffitt researchers shed light on how CAR T cells function mechanistically
February 16, 2019 - Female Anatomy May Play Big Role in Sperm’s Success
February 16, 2019 - BMI may mediate inverse link between fiber intake, knee OA
February 16, 2019 - Movement impairments in autism can be reversed through behavioral training
February 16, 2019 - Studies address racial disparities in postpartum period and cardiovascular health
February 16, 2019 - Scientists implicate hidden genes in the severity of autism symptoms
February 16, 2019 - Decreased deep sleep linked to early signs of Alzheimer’s disease
February 16, 2019 - Neuroscientists show how the brain responds to texture
February 16, 2019 - Gilead Announces Topline Data From Phase 3 STELLAR-4 Study of Selonsertib in Compensated Cirrhosis (F4) Due to Nonalcoholic Steatohepatitis (NASH)
February 16, 2019 - What Can I Do About Sweating? (for Teens)
February 16, 2019 - Companies navigate dementia conversations with older workers
February 16, 2019 - Newly developed stem cell technologies show promise for treating PD patients
February 16, 2019 - Collaborative material research could advance self-assembling nanomaterials
February 16, 2019 - Researchers take major step in creating technology that mimics the human brain
February 16, 2019 - Erasing memories associated with cocaine use reduces drug seeking behavior
February 16, 2019 - Artificial intelligence can accurately predict prognosis of ovarian cancer patients
February 16, 2019 - Racial disparities in cancer deaths on the decline for America
February 16, 2019 - FDA authorizes new interoperable insulin pump for children, adults with diabetes
February 16, 2019 - Coexisting Medical Conditions, Smoking Explain PTSD-CVD Link
February 16, 2019 - Skin Cancer Screening: MedlinePlus Lab Test Information
February 16, 2019 - ‘Happiness’ exercises can boost mood in those recovering from substance use disorder
February 16, 2019 - Cell manipulation could soon halt or reverse aging
February 16, 2019 - Pumped Breast Milk Falls Short of Breastfed Version
February 16, 2019 - Men’s porn habits could fuel partners’ eating disorders, study suggests
February 16, 2019 - Rapid progression of age-related diseases may result from formation of vicious cycles
February 16, 2019 - Immune checkpoint molecule protects against future development of cancer
February 16, 2019 - New method produces hydrogels that have properties similar to cells’ environment
February 16, 2019 - $4.1 million funding for heart research on Valentine’s Day
February 16, 2019 - General anesthesia in early infancy unlikely to have lasting effects on developing brains
February 16, 2019 - New breakthroughs for muscular dystrophy research
February 16, 2019 - First Opinion: Embryo editing for higher IQ is a fantasy. Embryo profiling for it is almost here
February 16, 2019 - Vapers develop cancer-related gene deregulation as cigarette smokers
February 16, 2019 - Bringing Antimicrobial Susceptibility Testing (AST) to the Community
February 16, 2019 - Decolonization protocol after hospital discharge can prevent dangerous infections
February 16, 2019 - Children with ASD more likely to face maltreatment, study finds
February 16, 2019 - Study finds genetic vulnerability to use of menthol cigarettes
February 16, 2019 - Promising drug developed to rejuvenate muscle cells
February 16, 2019 - H-RT should be the standard of care for men with low risk prostate cancer, study shows
February 16, 2019 - New technique using patients’ own modified cells could help treat Crohn’s disease
February 16, 2019 - Therapeutic endoscopy has an expanding role in the treatment of IBD
February 16, 2019 - Blood clot discovery could lead to development of better treatments for blood diseases
February 16, 2019 - Intervention can increase exclusive breastfeeding rates
February 16, 2019 - New project explores how gaming technologies can help cancer patients communicate better
February 16, 2019 - Catalyst Biosciences Presents Updated Data from Its Phase 2/3 Trial of Subcutaneous Marzeptacog Alfa (Activated) in Individuals with Hemophilia A or B with Inhibitors at the 12th Annual EAHAD Congress
February 16, 2019 - Rerouting nerves during amputation reduces phantom limb pain before it starts
February 16, 2019 - A Hormone Produced When We Exercise Might Help Fight Alzheimer’s
February 16, 2019 - Millions of British people breathe toxic air travelling to GPs
February 16, 2019 - Conformance of genetic characteristics found to be crucial for longer preservation of kidney graft
February 16, 2019 - Researchers use optogenetic tool to control, visualize receptor signals in neural cells
February 16, 2019 - New reversible antiplatelet therapy could reduce risk of blood clots, prevent cancer metastasis
February 16, 2019 - Testosterone is not the only hormone needed for penis development
February 16, 2019 - FDA Advisory Committee Recommends Approval of Spravato (esketamine) Nasal Spray for Adults with Treatment-Resistant Depression
February 15, 2019 - Heart surgery technology developed at Baptist Health debuts after years of secrecy
February 15, 2019 - Prescription Opioids Double Risk of Triggering Fatal Car Crash
February 15, 2019 - New study helps doctors better understand high blood pressure in pregnant women
February 15, 2019 - Beta wave control in Parkinson’s diseased brain could be a potential therapy
February 15, 2019 - Media representations of love may justify gender-based violence in young people
The hippocampus is crucial for forming non-hippocampal long-term memory during sleep

The hippocampus is crucial for forming non-hippocampal long-term memory during sleep

image_pdfDownload PDFimage_print
Effects of post-encoding sleep vs. wakefulness on memory in the NOR and OPR tasks. a) During the encoding phase of the two tasks rats explored two identical objects in the arena for 10 minutes. Encoding was followed by a two-hour interval in which the rat either slept or remained awake. Retrieval was tested immediately after the 2-hour post-encoding interval (recent memory), 1 week (NOR only) and 3 weeks later (remote memory). During retrieval, the rat explored the arena for 5 minutes. To test NOR retrieval one of the two objects (from the encoding phase) was replaced by a novel object (arrow). Recognition memory was indicated when the rat spent more time exploring the novel object than the familiar object – where exploration during the first minute was most sensitive for novelty exploration. To test OPR retrieval, one of the objects was displaced (relative to its location at encoding, arrow) and memory for the place was indicated when the rat spent more time exploring the displaced object than the stationary object. b) Mean + s.e.m. discrimination ratios during the first minute of exploration for NOR and OPR at the recent (2 hour) and remote (1 or 3 weeks after) retrieval tests. NOR memory benefited from post-encoding sleep (red bars: compared with wake – grey) only at the 3 weeks retrieval test, when NOR memory had decayed in the wake conditions. By contrast, OPR memory benefited from sleep at both recent and remote testing. Credit: Nature. doi:10.1038/s41586-018-0716-8

A longstanding division exists between hippocampus-dependent and non-hippocampus dependent memory since only the latter can be acquired and retrieved in the absence of normal hippocampal functions. In agreement with the familiar concept of “sleeping on a problem,” hippocampal-dependent memory consolidation is strongly supported by sleep. In a new study conducted by Anuck Sawangjit and colleagues, formation of long-term representation in a rat model of non-hippocampal memory was shown to depend on sleep and on the activation of hippocampus-dependent mechanisms during sleep.

The researchers used rats encoded with non-hippocampus-dependent (novel-object recognition, NOR) and hippocampus-dependent (object-place recognition, OPR) memories prior to a two-hour period of sleep and wakefulness. Memory was either tested immediately or remotely after one or three weeks. While object-place recognition memory was stronger for rats that had slept after encoding at both timepoints of testing, novel object-recognition memory only profited from sleep three weeks after encoding. At this timepoint (3 weeks), object-recognition memory was preserved in rats that slept after encoding and not in those that had been awake. Of note, intrahippocampal injection of muscimol (a psychoactive constituent) inactivated the hippocampus during post-encoding sleep, and abolished sleep-induced enhancement of remote novel-object recognition memory. In contrast, injection of muscimol before remote retrieval or memory encoding did not affect test performance, indicating that encoding and retrieval of novel-object recognition memory were hippocampus-independent. Consistent with the view that neuronal memory replay during slow-wave sleep contributed to long-term memory formation, remote novel-object memory recognition was associated with spindle activity during post-encoding slow-wave sleep in the study.

The distinction between hippocampus-dependent and non-hippocampus dependent forms of memory was highlighted in medical research when a patient underwent bilateral removal of large portions of the hippocampus and suffered severe anterograde amnesia. While encoding and retrieval of hippocampus-dependent memories required the hippocampus, this was not always the case for non-hippocampus dependent memory. Non-hippocampus dependent memory consisted of heterogenous memories, including motor skills and fear conditioning. Recent advances in memory research alongside the standard consolidation theory assume that episodes of memory were initially encoded into the hippocampal networks but redistributed across days, weeks and months to the neocortex for long-term storage during consolidated representation to thereby become independent of the hippocampus.

For hippocampus-dependent memory, an active systems consolidation process was proposed based on the findings that neural representation of freshly encoded memories replayed during subsequent slow-wave sleep (SWS). By mechanism, neural replay originating from hippocampus networks together with sharp-wave ripples and thalamic spindles promoted the transmission of memory information. Repetitive occurrence enabled their gradual redistribution in extrahippocampal networks. The role of sleep during non-hippocampus-dependent memory is less well understood.

**The hippocampus is crucial for forming non-hippocampal long-term memory during sleep
Verifying cannula location and muscimol spreading. a) Coronal brain section showing the location of cannula in the dorsal hippocampus (black arrow) with the position of guide cannula in the overlying cortex. b) Coronal brain section showing the spread of muscimol (red) after infusion into the hippocampus. The infusion protocol was similar in the behavioral experiments. After implantation of the guide cannula into the dorsal hippocampus, animals were infused with fluorophore conjugated muscimol. Animals were intracardially perfused after infusion and brains post-fixed in paraformaldehyde (4%) for 24 hours. Brains were sliced with a vibratome to obtain sections for histopathology and fluorescent-microscopy imaging. Credit: Nature, doi:10.1038/s41586-018-0716-8

In the study, Sawangjit et al, compared the effects of post-encoding wakefulness on consolidation of non-hippocampus-dependent and hippocampus-dependent forms of memory in rats to understand the evolution of consolidation effects with time. To accomplish this, the researchers used the novel-object recognition (NOR) task and object-place recognition (OPR) task as tests to determine non-hippocampus-dependent and hippocampus-dependent-memory. Performance on the NOR task typically relied on the perirhinal cortex, normal hippocampal functions were not necessary to encode or retrieve NOR memory in rats according to preceding studies. During a two-hour interval, the rats either slept or remained awake after task encoding. Retrieval was tested immediately after the two-hour interval or conducted after one week (NOR only), or three weeks later (remote tests) to test long-term memory. The NOR memory did not differ between sleep and awake states, but in contrast OPR memory significantly enhanced after sleep only and not while awake. Repeated reactivation of newly encoded hippocampal representation during subsequent slow-wave-sleep (SWV) had an impact on the consolidating effect of sleep on hippocampus-dependent spatial memory.

Previous evidence also suggested that hippocampus-dependent and non-hippocampus-dependent memory systems interacted during consolidation. The researchers therefore investigated if hippocampal activity critically contributed to the integration of non-hippocampus-dependent memory by reversibly inactivating the function of the hippocampus. For this, the rats were infused with muscimol (a psychoactive constituent) into the dorsal hippocampus during sleep after encoding the NOR task.

The effects of reversibly inactivating the hippocampus on NOR memory. Left, procedures; right, mean + s.e.m. discrimination ratios, with overlaid dot plots. a) To suppress hippocampal activity, muscimol was bilaterally infused (for 2 min) into the dorsal hippocampus, either during the post-encoding interval upon the first occurrence of continuous SWS (top), or 15 min before remote retrieval testing 3 weeks after encoding (bottom). Hippocampal inactivation during post-encoding sleep (red bar) abolished remote NOR memory whereas inactivation before retrieval testing (blue bar) was ineffective. Grey bar, vehicle injection. b) Muscimol (purple bar) was infused shortly after encoding while the rats remained awake during the 2-h post-encoding interval. Retrieval was tested 1 week later. Compared with untreated wake control rats (empty bar), which had intact hippocampal function and stayed awake during the post-encoding interval, hippocampal inactivation did not disturb but enhanced NOR performance. Timing (with reference to encoding), dosage and procedures of muscimol infusion were the same as in (a). c) Muscimol (or vehicle) was infused 15 min before retrieval testing of recent NOR memory (top) or 15 min before the encoding phase (bottom). Retrieval was tested 30 min after encoding (rats stayed awake during this interval). Hippocampal inactivation does not affect retrieval of recent NOR memory either during retrieval or during encoding. Credit: Nature, doi:10.1038/s41586-018-0716-8.

Three weeks after, during remote retrieval testing, rats who had received muscimol injection into the hippocampus during sleep after learning did not show significant NOR memory. Comparatively, remote NOR memory was preserved in animals injected with the vehicle at the same timepoint. The result demonstrated that the hippocampus was crucial to form persistent NOR memory during sleep. Previous discrepancies of the hypothesis were thus resolved by the new results observed in the study, to show that the formation of long-term NOR memory relied on a hippocampal mechanism that was specifically active during sleep. Combined results confirmed that persistent long-term NOR memory formation relied on a hippocampal mechanism that was specifically active during sleep.

In the architecture of post-encoding sleep, results were comparable to previous studies, and analyses revealed that remote NOR memory retrieval strongly correlated with measures of spindle activity during slow-wave-sleep (SWS). The combined results agreed with the idea that muscimol prevented long-term memory formation by suppressing hippocampal ripples as well as its associated reactivation during SWS. The findings by Sawangjit et al. did not completely rule out contributions of REM-sleep-related mechanisms, although the concept was not addressed in the present study.

Contribution of post-encoding slow wave sleep to remote NOR memory. Contribution of post-encoding slow wave sleep to remote NOR memory. a) NOR performance (discrimination ratio) at the 3-week retrieval test correlated with the number of sleep spindles during SWS (top) and spindle mean duration during the 2-h post-encoding interval (middle), as well as with spindle mean duration during the first 30 min of post-encoding sleep (bottom). b) Intrahippocampal local field potentials (LFPs) were recorded in additional rats to examine the effects of bilateral intrahippocampal infusion of muscimol (Mus) (versus vehicle, Veh) on (from left to right) ripples, spindles, and slow oscillations (SO) in hippocampal networks during SWS. Muscimol decreased the total number and density of ripples, as well as spindle power and slow oscillation amplitude. Credit: Nature, doi:10.1038/s41586-018-0716-8.

While ample evidence already exists to indicate involvement of the hippocampus during memory consolidation for their retention and retrieval with normal hippocampal function. New results in the study indicated necessity of the normal hippocampus to form persistent long-term representations on a task that is classified non-hippocampus-dependent. To understand how the hippocampus contributed to long-term NOR memory formation, the researchers proposed that during sleep the hippocampus likely boosted object representation by activating context-related representations instead of direct perirhinal object memory enhancement. This proposal was supported by the observed correlation of long-term NOR performance with post-encoding sleep spindle activity.

Comprehensive findings of the study suggested a common hippocampal mechanism that boosted consolidation in both hippocampus-dependent and non-hippocampus-dependent memory systems by reactivating contextual features. Since non-hippocampal-dependent memory is heterogenous (motor skills, fear conditioning etc.), other memories of the nature require investigations so as to support the proposed general hippocampal mechanism of long-term memory formation.


Explore further:
Nested sequences: An indispensable mechanism for forming memories

More information:
Anuck Sawangjit et al. The hippocampus is crucial for forming non-hippocampal long-term memory during sleep, Nature (2018). DOI: 10.1038/s41586-018-0716-8

Robert Stickgold. Sleep-dependent memory consolidation, Nature (2005). DOI: 10.1038/nature04286

Giulio Tononi et al. Sleep and the Price of Plasticity: From Synaptic and Cellular Homeostasis to Memory Consolidation and Integration, Neuron (2014). DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.12.025

Journal reference:
Nature

Neuron

Tagged with:

About author

Related Articles