Breaking News
May 3, 2019 - Vaping and Smoking May Signal Greater Motivation to Quit
May 3, 2019 - Dementia looks different in brains of Hispanics
May 3, 2019 - Short-Staffed Nursing Homes See Drop In Medicare Ratings
May 3, 2019 - Study of teens with eating disorders explores how substance users differ from non-substance users
May 3, 2019 - Scientists develop new video game that may help in the study of Alzheimer’s
May 3, 2019 - Arc Bio introduces Galileo Pathogen Solution product line at ASM Clinical Virology Symposium
May 3, 2019 - Cornell University study uncovers relationship between starch digestion gene and gut bacteria
May 3, 2019 - How to Safely Use Glucose Meters and Test Strips for Diabetes
May 3, 2019 - Anti-inflammatory drugs ineffective for prevention of Alzheimer’s disease
May 3, 2019 - Study tracks Pennsylvania’s oil and gas waste-disposal practices
May 3, 2019 - Creating a better radiation diagnostic test for astronauts
May 3, 2019 - Vegans are often deficient in these four nutrients
May 3, 2019 - PPDC announces seed grants to develop medical devices for children
May 3, 2019 - Study maps out the frequency and impact of water polo head injuries
May 3, 2019 - Research on Reddit identifies risks associated with unproven treatments for opioid addiction
May 3, 2019 - Good smells may help ease tobacco cravings
May 3, 2019 - Medical financial hardship found to be very common among people in the United States
May 3, 2019 - Researchers develop multimodal system for personalized post-stroke rehabilitation
May 3, 2019 - Study shows significant mortality benefit with CABG over percutaneous coronary intervention
May 3, 2019 - Will gene-editing of human embryos ever be justifiable?
May 3, 2019 - FDA Approves Dengvaxia (dengue vaccine) for the Prevention of Dengue Disease in Endemic Regions
May 3, 2019 - Why Tonsillitis Keeps Coming Back
May 3, 2019 - Fighting the opioid epidemic with data
May 3, 2019 - Maggot sausages may soon be a reality
May 3, 2019 - Deletion of ATDC gene prevents development of pancreatic cancer in mice
May 2, 2019 - Targeted Therapy Promising for Rare Hematologic Cancer
May 2, 2019 - Alzheimer’s disease is a ‘double-prion disorder,’ study shows
May 2, 2019 - Reservoir bugs: How one bacterial menace makes its home in the human stomach
May 2, 2019 - Clinical, Admin Staff From Cardiology Get Sneak Peek at Epic
May 2, 2019 - Depression increases hospital use and mortality in children
May 2, 2019 - Vicon and NOC support CURE International to create first gait lab in Ethiopia
May 2, 2019 - Researchers use 3D printer to make paper organs
May 2, 2019 - Viral infection in utero associated with behavioral abnormalities in offspring
May 2, 2019 - U.S. Teen Opioid Deaths Soaring
May 2, 2019 - Opioid distribution data should be public
May 2, 2019 - In the Spotlight: “I’m learning every single day”
May 2, 2019 - 2019 Schaefer Scholars Announced
May 2, 2019 - Podcast: KHN’s ‘What The Health?’ Bye-Bye, ACA, And Hello ‘Medicare-For-All’?
May 2, 2019 - Study describes new viral molecular evasion mechanism used by cytomegalovirus
May 2, 2019 - SLU study suggests a more equitable way for Medicare reimbursement
May 2, 2019 - Scientists discover first gene involved in lower urinary tract obstruction
May 2, 2019 - Researchers identify 34 genes associated with increased risk of ovarian cancer
May 2, 2019 - Many low-income infants receive formula in the first few days of life, finds study
May 2, 2019 - Global study finds high success rate for hip and knee replacements
May 2, 2019 - Taking depression seriously: What is it?
May 2, 2019 - With Head Injuries Mounting, Will Cities Put Their Feet Down On E-Scooters?
May 2, 2019 - Scientists develop small fluorophores for tracking metabolites in living cells
May 2, 2019 - Study casts new light into how mothers’ and babies’ genes influence birth weight
May 2, 2019 - Researchers uncover new brain mechanisms regulating body weight
May 2, 2019 - Organ-on-chip systems offered to Asia-Pacific regions by Sydney’s AXT
May 2, 2019 - Adoption of new rules drops readmission penalties against safety net hospitals
May 2, 2019 - Kids and teens who consume zero-calorie sweetened beverages do not save calories
May 2, 2019 - Improved procedure for cancer-related erectile dysfunction
May 2, 2019 - Hormone may improve social behavior in autism
May 2, 2019 - Alzheimer’s disease may be caused by infectious proteins called prions
May 2, 2019 - Even Doctors Can’t Navigate Our ‘Broken Health Care System’
May 2, 2019 - Study looks at the impact on criminal persistence of head injuries
May 2, 2019 - Honey ‘as high in sugars as table sugar’
May 2, 2019 - Innovations to U.S. food system could help consumers in choosing healthy foods
May 2, 2019 - FDA Approves Mavyret (glecaprevir and pibrentasvir) as First Treatment for All Genotypes of Hepatitis C in Pediatric Patients
May 2, 2019 - Women underreport prevalence and intensity of their own snoring
May 2, 2019 - Concussion summit focuses on science behind brain injury
May 2, 2019 - Booker’s Argument For Environmental Justice Stays Within The Lines
May 2, 2019 - Cornell research explains increased metastatic cancer risk in diabetics
May 2, 2019 - Mount Sinai study provides fresh insights into cellular pathways that cause cancer
May 2, 2019 - Researchers to study link between prenatal pesticide exposures and childhood ADHD
May 2, 2019 - CoGEN Congress 2019: Speakers’ overviews
May 2, 2019 - A new strategy for managing diabetic macular edema in people with good vision
May 2, 2019 - Sagent Pharmaceuticals Issues Voluntary Nationwide Recall of Ketorolac Tromethamine Injection, USP, 60mg/2mL (30mg per mL) Due to Lack of Sterility Assurance
May 2, 2019 - Screen time associated with behavioral problems in preschoolers
May 2, 2019 - Hormone reduces social impairment in kids with autism | News Center
May 2, 2019 - Researchers synthesize peroxidase-mimicking nanozyme with low cost and superior catalytic activity
May 2, 2019 - Study results of a potential drug to treat Type 2 diabetes in children announced
May 2, 2019 - Multigene test helps doctors to make effective treatment decisions for breast cancer patients
May 2, 2019 - UNC School of Medicine initiative providing unique care to dementia patients
May 2, 2019 - Nestlé Health Science and VHP join forces to launch innovative COPES program for cancer patients
May 2, 2019 - Study examines how our brain generates consciousness and loses it during anesthesia
May 2, 2019 - Transition Support Program May Aid Young Adults With Type 1 Diabetes
May 2, 2019 - Study shows how neutrophils exacerbate atherosclerosis by inducing smooth muscle-cell death
May 2, 2019 - Research reveals complexity of how we make decisions
The hippocampus is crucial for forming non-hippocampal long-term memory during sleep

The hippocampus is crucial for forming non-hippocampal long-term memory during sleep

Effects of post-encoding sleep vs. wakefulness on memory in the NOR and OPR tasks. a) During the encoding phase of the two tasks rats explored two identical objects in the arena for 10 minutes. Encoding was followed by a two-hour interval in which the rat either slept or remained awake. Retrieval was tested immediately after the 2-hour post-encoding interval (recent memory), 1 week (NOR only) and 3 weeks later (remote memory). During retrieval, the rat explored the arena for 5 minutes. To test NOR retrieval one of the two objects (from the encoding phase) was replaced by a novel object (arrow). Recognition memory was indicated when the rat spent more time exploring the novel object than the familiar object – where exploration during the first minute was most sensitive for novelty exploration. To test OPR retrieval, one of the objects was displaced (relative to its location at encoding, arrow) and memory for the place was indicated when the rat spent more time exploring the displaced object than the stationary object. b) Mean + s.e.m. discrimination ratios during the first minute of exploration for NOR and OPR at the recent (2 hour) and remote (1 or 3 weeks after) retrieval tests. NOR memory benefited from post-encoding sleep (red bars: compared with wake – grey) only at the 3 weeks retrieval test, when NOR memory had decayed in the wake conditions. By contrast, OPR memory benefited from sleep at both recent and remote testing. Credit: Nature. doi:10.1038/s41586-018-0716-8

A longstanding division exists between hippocampus-dependent and non-hippocampus dependent memory since only the latter can be acquired and retrieved in the absence of normal hippocampal functions. In agreement with the familiar concept of “sleeping on a problem,” hippocampal-dependent memory consolidation is strongly supported by sleep. In a new study conducted by Anuck Sawangjit and colleagues, formation of long-term representation in a rat model of non-hippocampal memory was shown to depend on sleep and on the activation of hippocampus-dependent mechanisms during sleep.

The researchers used rats encoded with non-hippocampus-dependent (novel-object recognition, NOR) and hippocampus-dependent (object-place recognition, OPR) memories prior to a two-hour period of sleep and wakefulness. Memory was either tested immediately or remotely after one or three weeks. While object-place recognition memory was stronger for rats that had slept after encoding at both timepoints of testing, novel object-recognition memory only profited from sleep three weeks after encoding. At this timepoint (3 weeks), object-recognition memory was preserved in rats that slept after encoding and not in those that had been awake. Of note, intrahippocampal injection of muscimol (a psychoactive constituent) inactivated the hippocampus during post-encoding sleep, and abolished sleep-induced enhancement of remote novel-object recognition memory. In contrast, injection of muscimol before remote retrieval or memory encoding did not affect test performance, indicating that encoding and retrieval of novel-object recognition memory were hippocampus-independent. Consistent with the view that neuronal memory replay during slow-wave sleep contributed to long-term memory formation, remote novel-object memory recognition was associated with spindle activity during post-encoding slow-wave sleep in the study.

The distinction between hippocampus-dependent and non-hippocampus dependent forms of memory was highlighted in medical research when a patient underwent bilateral removal of large portions of the hippocampus and suffered severe anterograde amnesia. While encoding and retrieval of hippocampus-dependent memories required the hippocampus, this was not always the case for non-hippocampus dependent memory. Non-hippocampus dependent memory consisted of heterogenous memories, including motor skills and fear conditioning. Recent advances in memory research alongside the standard consolidation theory assume that episodes of memory were initially encoded into the hippocampal networks but redistributed across days, weeks and months to the neocortex for long-term storage during consolidated representation to thereby become independent of the hippocampus.

For hippocampus-dependent memory, an active systems consolidation process was proposed based on the findings that neural representation of freshly encoded memories replayed during subsequent slow-wave sleep (SWS). By mechanism, neural replay originating from hippocampus networks together with sharp-wave ripples and thalamic spindles promoted the transmission of memory information. Repetitive occurrence enabled their gradual redistribution in extrahippocampal networks. The role of sleep during non-hippocampus-dependent memory is less well understood.

**The hippocampus is crucial for forming non-hippocampal long-term memory during sleep
Verifying cannula location and muscimol spreading. a) Coronal brain section showing the location of cannula in the dorsal hippocampus (black arrow) with the position of guide cannula in the overlying cortex. b) Coronal brain section showing the spread of muscimol (red) after infusion into the hippocampus. The infusion protocol was similar in the behavioral experiments. After implantation of the guide cannula into the dorsal hippocampus, animals were infused with fluorophore conjugated muscimol. Animals were intracardially perfused after infusion and brains post-fixed in paraformaldehyde (4%) for 24 hours. Brains were sliced with a vibratome to obtain sections for histopathology and fluorescent-microscopy imaging. Credit: Nature, doi:10.1038/s41586-018-0716-8

In the study, Sawangjit et al, compared the effects of post-encoding wakefulness on consolidation of non-hippocampus-dependent and hippocampus-dependent forms of memory in rats to understand the evolution of consolidation effects with time. To accomplish this, the researchers used the novel-object recognition (NOR) task and object-place recognition (OPR) task as tests to determine non-hippocampus-dependent and hippocampus-dependent-memory. Performance on the NOR task typically relied on the perirhinal cortex, normal hippocampal functions were not necessary to encode or retrieve NOR memory in rats according to preceding studies. During a two-hour interval, the rats either slept or remained awake after task encoding. Retrieval was tested immediately after the two-hour interval or conducted after one week (NOR only), or three weeks later (remote tests) to test long-term memory. The NOR memory did not differ between sleep and awake states, but in contrast OPR memory significantly enhanced after sleep only and not while awake. Repeated reactivation of newly encoded hippocampal representation during subsequent slow-wave-sleep (SWV) had an impact on the consolidating effect of sleep on hippocampus-dependent spatial memory.

Previous evidence also suggested that hippocampus-dependent and non-hippocampus-dependent memory systems interacted during consolidation. The researchers therefore investigated if hippocampal activity critically contributed to the integration of non-hippocampus-dependent memory by reversibly inactivating the function of the hippocampus. For this, the rats were infused with muscimol (a psychoactive constituent) into the dorsal hippocampus during sleep after encoding the NOR task.

The effects of reversibly inactivating the hippocampus on NOR memory. Left, procedures; right, mean + s.e.m. discrimination ratios, with overlaid dot plots. a) To suppress hippocampal activity, muscimol was bilaterally infused (for 2 min) into the dorsal hippocampus, either during the post-encoding interval upon the first occurrence of continuous SWS (top), or 15 min before remote retrieval testing 3 weeks after encoding (bottom). Hippocampal inactivation during post-encoding sleep (red bar) abolished remote NOR memory whereas inactivation before retrieval testing (blue bar) was ineffective. Grey bar, vehicle injection. b) Muscimol (purple bar) was infused shortly after encoding while the rats remained awake during the 2-h post-encoding interval. Retrieval was tested 1 week later. Compared with untreated wake control rats (empty bar), which had intact hippocampal function and stayed awake during the post-encoding interval, hippocampal inactivation did not disturb but enhanced NOR performance. Timing (with reference to encoding), dosage and procedures of muscimol infusion were the same as in (a). c) Muscimol (or vehicle) was infused 15 min before retrieval testing of recent NOR memory (top) or 15 min before the encoding phase (bottom). Retrieval was tested 30 min after encoding (rats stayed awake during this interval). Hippocampal inactivation does not affect retrieval of recent NOR memory either during retrieval or during encoding. Credit: Nature, doi:10.1038/s41586-018-0716-8.

Three weeks after, during remote retrieval testing, rats who had received muscimol injection into the hippocampus during sleep after learning did not show significant NOR memory. Comparatively, remote NOR memory was preserved in animals injected with the vehicle at the same timepoint. The result demonstrated that the hippocampus was crucial to form persistent NOR memory during sleep. Previous discrepancies of the hypothesis were thus resolved by the new results observed in the study, to show that the formation of long-term NOR memory relied on a hippocampal mechanism that was specifically active during sleep. Combined results confirmed that persistent long-term NOR memory formation relied on a hippocampal mechanism that was specifically active during sleep.

In the architecture of post-encoding sleep, results were comparable to previous studies, and analyses revealed that remote NOR memory retrieval strongly correlated with measures of spindle activity during slow-wave-sleep (SWS). The combined results agreed with the idea that muscimol prevented long-term memory formation by suppressing hippocampal ripples as well as its associated reactivation during SWS. The findings by Sawangjit et al. did not completely rule out contributions of REM-sleep-related mechanisms, although the concept was not addressed in the present study.

Contribution of post-encoding slow wave sleep to remote NOR memory. Contribution of post-encoding slow wave sleep to remote NOR memory. a) NOR performance (discrimination ratio) at the 3-week retrieval test correlated with the number of sleep spindles during SWS (top) and spindle mean duration during the 2-h post-encoding interval (middle), as well as with spindle mean duration during the first 30 min of post-encoding sleep (bottom). b) Intrahippocampal local field potentials (LFPs) were recorded in additional rats to examine the effects of bilateral intrahippocampal infusion of muscimol (Mus) (versus vehicle, Veh) on (from left to right) ripples, spindles, and slow oscillations (SO) in hippocampal networks during SWS. Muscimol decreased the total number and density of ripples, as well as spindle power and slow oscillation amplitude. Credit: Nature, doi:10.1038/s41586-018-0716-8.

While ample evidence already exists to indicate involvement of the hippocampus during memory consolidation for their retention and retrieval with normal hippocampal function. New results in the study indicated necessity of the normal hippocampus to form persistent long-term representations on a task that is classified non-hippocampus-dependent. To understand how the hippocampus contributed to long-term NOR memory formation, the researchers proposed that during sleep the hippocampus likely boosted object representation by activating context-related representations instead of direct perirhinal object memory enhancement. This proposal was supported by the observed correlation of long-term NOR performance with post-encoding sleep spindle activity.

Comprehensive findings of the study suggested a common hippocampal mechanism that boosted consolidation in both hippocampus-dependent and non-hippocampus-dependent memory systems by reactivating contextual features. Since non-hippocampal-dependent memory is heterogenous (motor skills, fear conditioning etc.), other memories of the nature require investigations so as to support the proposed general hippocampal mechanism of long-term memory formation.


Explore further:
Nested sequences: An indispensable mechanism for forming memories

More information:
Anuck Sawangjit et al. The hippocampus is crucial for forming non-hippocampal long-term memory during sleep, Nature (2018). DOI: 10.1038/s41586-018-0716-8

Robert Stickgold. Sleep-dependent memory consolidation, Nature (2005). DOI: 10.1038/nature04286

Giulio Tononi et al. Sleep and the Price of Plasticity: From Synaptic and Cellular Homeostasis to Memory Consolidation and Integration, Neuron (2014). DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.12.025

Journal reference:
Nature

Neuron

Tagged with:

About author

Related Articles